DEFECTIVE COLOR VISION
AND TRAFFIC SIGNALS

KINDS AND EFFECTS OF DEFECTIVE COLOR VISION

These are the major types of color vision defects:

  • Protanopia - The loss of the red-sensitive cells

    A person with protanopia sees in tints and shades of the colors yellow and blue. Red objects look very dark.

  • Deuteranopia - The loss of the green-sensitive cells

    A person with deuteranopia sees in tints and shades of the colors yellow and blue. Green objects are slightly darker than normal.

  • Tritanopia - The loss of the blue-sensitive cells

    A person with tritanopia sees in tints and shades of the colors red and green. Blue objects look dark.

  • Tetartanopia - Undocumented loss of sensitivity to yellow (see inset)
  • Protanomaly - The red sensitive cells are sensitive to leaf green (yellow green) instead of red

    A person with protanomaly sees full color, but some oranges, yellows, greens, and browns are seen as the wrong color.

  • Deuteranomaly - The green sensitive cells are sensitive to yellow instead

    A person with deuteranomaly sees full color, but some oranges, yellows, greens, and browns are seen as the wrong color.

  • Tritanomaly - The blue sensitive cells are sensitive to cyan instead

    A person with tritanomaly sees full color, but some magentas, violets, blues, cyans, and greens are seen as the wrong color.

  • Red-Green Indistinction - No red-green differentiation but no sensitivity changes

    A person with red-green indistinction sees in tints and shades of yellow and blue. All objects are the correct brightness (no change in spectral sensitivity). Causes include missing bipolar cells differentiating red and green, or red and green pigments mixed in the same kind of cone cell.

  • Red-Green anomaly - Both the red sensitive cells and the green sensitive cells respond to the wrong colors.

    A person with red-green anomaly sees color, but some reds, oranges, yellows, greens, and browns are seen as the wrong color.

    Red-green anomaly is protanomaly and deuteranomaly in the same person.

  • Cone Monochromatism - Only the blue sensitive cells and the rods work

    A person with cone monochromatism sees the world in black and white. Green and yellow appear dark, and red appears black.

  • Rod monochromatism - Only the rods work

    A person with rod monochromatism sees the world in black and white. Reds appear black. The person can not see well in bright light.

For more on this, see Human Color Vision Defects.

Defective color vision

DEFECTIVE COLOR VISION

Outer ring:
     Normal color vision

Second ring:
     Protanopia

Third ring:
     Deuteranopia

Inner ring:
     Tritanopia


More Definitions

  • Protan - Refers to any defect in the red cone, including protanopia and protanomaly
  • Deutan - Refers to any defect in the green cone, including deuteranopia and deuteranomaly.
  • Tritan - Refers to any defect in the blue cone, including tritanopia and tritanomaly.

Tetartanopia is very rare, if it exists at all. It might be a failure of the bipolar cells for blue-yellow differentiation. Or it might have been an attempt to provide a missing disease that the Hering Opponent Color theory predicted.

AN AID THE DRIVER CAN USE FOR ANOMALOUS TRICHROMATIC VISION

For people with anomalous trichromatic vision (protanomaly, deuteranomaly, or tritanomaly), a special solution now exists. It is a set of glasses with notch filters that remove spectral yellow and spectral cyan from the light entering the eye. The remaining light usually has the necessary information for correctly identifying most colors, but without interference from the anomaly.

A company named EnChroma makes these glasses.

Traffic signals and signs should appear the correct color to anyone with anomalous trichromatic vision wearing these glasses.

Note that these glasses do not work for dichromatic vision (protanopia, deuteranopia, tritanopia, or red-green indistinction) or monochromatic vision (single cone type or cone blindness).

One caveat is that the light from a low-pressure sodium streetlight (monochromatic yellow) is rendered totally invisible by these glasses. If this happens, the glasses must be removed while the driver is driving under low-pressure sodium streetlights.

DESIGNING TRAFFIC SIGNALS TO COMPENSATE FOR DEFECTIVE COLOR VISION

Because recognizing the aspect of the traffic signal is so important to traffic safety, special engineering is needed to ensure that drivers with defective color vision receive the correct message. Several methods are used:

  1. Position of the lamp in the signal face.
  2. Selection of colors that can still be distinguished from each other by most people with defective color vision.
  3. Other methods, such as different shapes for different colors.

Positioning the lamps in the signal face:

The US Department of Transportation sets requirements in its Manual on Uniform Traffic Control Devices (MUTCD). The following are requirements for positioning of lamps within the signal face, intended to aid people with defective color vision to determine the correct signal aspect:

  • Each signal face must have 3, 4, or 5 lamps.
  • All red lamps must be above all other lamps in vertical faces and cluster faces.
  • All red lamps must be to the left of all other lamps in horizontal faces.
  • In vertical and cluster faces, each yellow lamp must be directly above its corresponding green lamp.
  • In horizontal faces, each yellow arrow must be directly to the left of its green arrow.
  • A flashing yellow arrow for permissive turns must be between the yellow arrow and the green arrow.
  • Green indications never flash.

Color of the signal lamps:

The US Department of Transportation also sets requirements for the colors of traffic signal indications. The following are the specification for the colors of signal indications:

  •                               The red signal must give off some orange light, but no green or blue light.

  •                               The yellow signal must give off some orange and green light, but no blue light.

  •                               The green signal must give off some blue light, but no yellow or red light.

  •                               The orange pedestrian signal must give off some red and yellow light, but no blue light.

  •                               The white pedestrian signal must give off more blue than orange.

With Light Emitting Diode (LED) signals, these colors have finally been produced. In the days of incandescent lamps, this standard was easier stated than achieved. The signals, especially the green ones, gave off colors they were not supposed to. But the technology was not there to produce the correct colors. These were the best colors available:

  •           The red leaks yellow and white.

  •           The yellow leaks white.

  •           The green leaks white.


The following table shows how each signal lamp looks to people with the most common color defects:

Signal colorProtanopiaDeuteranopia TritanopiaRed-Green IndistinctionMonochromatism


















The signal colors are usually easily distinguished.

The shapes on the pedestrian signals remove the need for color identification.


Two problems remain:

  • A person with deuteranopia has trouble distinguishing red from yellow, because only the brightness is different. This is not usually a problem when the signals are cycling normally. The yellow light stays on for at most 6 seconds, then the red. But there are several places where this can be a problem:
    1. The signal is on night flashing, and the signal face is invisible, because the government decided a dark color looked better for the signal faces (aesthetics before safety). It is very hard to tell a flashing red light from a flashing yellow light without being able to see the position of the light in the signal face.
    2. Single section flashing beacons are used. But there is supposed to be a stop sign posted if the beacon is red, so this is a problem only if a jurisdiction does not follow the standards.
    3. There is almost no visible distinction between a flashing yellow arrow and a flashing red arrow. Both require yielding, but the flashing red arrow also requires a stop. With a normal signal face, the position of the arrow is an indication. A flashing red arrow would be the top indication, while a flashing yellow arrow would be on the bottom or next to the bottom. Again, this is hard to determine at night.

    Examine the diagrams on the right:

    • The upper diagram shows how the 10 standard indications, as defined in the new 2009 Manual on Uniform Traffic Control Devices (MUTCD, US Department of Transportation), appear to most people.
    • The lower diagram shows how a person with deuteranopia sees these indications. Notice how the only difference between a red indication and a yellow indication is brightness.

      Note that only the signal indications have been color-corrected for deuteranopia, not the housings or the background.

    The red and yellow are hard to separate visually. This remains a problem, because deuteranopia is the most common form of dichromatism.
     

  • The problem is worse for the person with monochromatism, because the red is very dark if it is visible at all.
Standard signals

The 10 Standard Indications
 

Deuteranopia view

As Seen With Deuteranopia


Other Methods:

The MUTCD does not allow most of these other methods:

  • Eastern Canada has used different shapes for different colors of traffic signals.
    • Red - square
    • Yellow - diamond
    • green - circle

    But what do they do with arrows?

    No more of these are being installed. As they become unserviceable, they are being replaced with standard signals. It cost too much to have the special faces made.

  • South Korea also has a design with different shapes for different colors of traffic signals. Unfortunately, the shapes are not the same as those on the Canadian signal.
    • Red - triangle
    • Yellow - circle
    • green - square

    Again, what do they do with arrows?

    One source of error might be that the triangle is perceived as an arrow.

    Only one or two of these have been installed.

    PLEASE! Only one standard!!! Not two!!!

  • Another trick from Canada is to put a red light at each end of the signal face.
  • Older signals in the US used two red lights. Though this was originally for burnout protection, it also told people with deuteranopia when the red was on.
  • Since red and yellow are the hardest for people with deuteranopia to distinguish, it normally helps that the yellow light does not stay on for more than 6 seconds. The problem is when the light is flashing at night. A dark colored signal face prevents this person from seeing which lamp is lit. So signal faces should be yellow.
  • A single flashing red light with no other lamps must always be accompanied by a stop sign in the US. The stop sign identifies it.
  • Flashing beacons with two lamps used to alternate. But the new rules prevent this, removing this clue that a flashing beacon is there, not a traffic light on night flash.
  • A new LED signal in Japan has a magenta X on an otherwise red lamp. The X looks bluish to someone with deuteranopia. But since no red or yellow arrows exist in Japan, they didn't tackle the arrow problem.

    Note that, since Japan drives on the left, the red is on the right end of the horizontal signal.

Shapes traffic signal

Canada Shapes Signal
 

Korean shapes traffic signal

Korean Signal
 

Double red traffic signal

Canada Double Red Signal
 

Japan magenta X

Japanese Signal
 

A NEW SOLUTION

The new solution combines the Japanese magenta X in the red signal (above) with a modification of the red arrow:

  • The circular red signal has a magenta X in it. It is seen as a totally different color by people with protanopia or deuteranopia.
  • The stem of the red arrow is also magenta, making it appear a different color than the color of the barbs.
  • The other signals are the standard colors prescribed in the MUTCD.

Examine the diagrams below. Note that the signals are easily recognized in all cases:

  1. In the case of protanopia, the red signals are much more visible, because the X and the arrow stem stand out against the dimmer remainder of the lighted portion.
  2. In the case of deuteranopia, the red signals are two different colors, with the X and the arrow stem looking blue, while the rest of the lighted portion appears yellow.
  3. In the case of tritanopia, the X and the stem of the arrow are not seen as different from the rest of the red signal. But it does not matter, because the three signal colors are quite distinctly different in tritanopia.
  4. All of the signals are distinctly different colors in all cases of anomalous color vision.
  5. The red signal is even distinctly different for people with monochromatism, because the X and the stem of the arrow are brighter than the rest of the lighted portion. Look at the diagram for protanopia to get the idea.
  6. The person with normal vision barely notices the change in color in the red signal.
Modified signals

Colorblind Signal, Normal Vision
 

Protanopia view

As Seen With Protanopia
 

Deuteranopia view

As Seen With Deuteranopia
 

Tritanopia view

As Seen With Tritanopia
 

The diagrams here are derived from a diagram originally used on one of the page author's other web pages. The text in the diagrams pertains to material on the other page.

Links: