THE BASIC NORMAL HUMAN COLOR VISION SYSTEM
The visible Spectrum
The visible spectrum is the portion of the electromagnetic spectrum with wavelengths between
380 nm and 760 nm. These frequencies are sensed as the following colors (wavelengths are the
centers of the colors):
- 400 nm violet
- 440 nm blue
- 490 nm cyan
- 530 nm green
- 575 nm yellow
- 590 nm orange
- 650 nm red
Four kinds of sensors:
Use the chart of light sensitivity curves at right.
-
Rods
Rods are primarily used at night, because they are much more sensitive than the cones. But
they also provide motion and edge detection in brighter light. They are sensitive to a range
of wavelengths between 380 nm and 650 nm, peaking at 530 nm. This covers the colors violet,
blue, cyan, green, yellow, and orange. But rods are encoded as white, not as any specific
color.
-
Blue Sensitive Cones
The blue sensitive cones are sensitive to a range of wavelengths between 380 nm and 550 nm,
peaking at 450 nm. This includes violet, blue, and cyan light.
-
Green Sensitive Cones
The green sensitive cones are sensitive to a range of wavelengths between 430 nm and 670 nm,
peaking at 550 nm. This includes cyan, green, yellow, and orange light.
-
Red Sensitive Cones
The red sensitive cones are sensitive to two ranges of wavelengths. The major range is
between 500 nm and 760 nm, peaking at 600 nm. This includes green, yellow, orange, and red
light. The minor range is between 380 nm and 450 nm, peaking at 430 nm. This includes violet
and some blue. The minor range is what makes the hues appear to form a circle instead of a
straight line.
Seeing other colors:
- Violet activates the blue cone, and
partially activates the red cone.
- Blue activates the blue cone.
- Cyan activates the blue cone and the green
cone.
- Green activates the green cone, and slightly
activates the red and blue cones.
- Yellow activates the green cone and the
red cone.
- Orange activates the red cone, and partially
activates the green cone.
- Red activates the red cone.
- Magenta activates the red cone and the
blue cone.
- White activates the red cone, the green
cone, and the blue cone.
Colors not listed here are seen due to varying strengths of light activating the red, green,
and blue cones.
Note that some people with protanomaly, deuteranomaly, or tritanomaly may see the correct
colors, rather than the distorted colors they normally see, when viewing the above colors on
a 3-color monitor.
After the cones have received the colors, the color information is encoded in the bipolar and
ganglion cells in the retina before it is passed on to the brain. Three different encodings are
used:
- The primary encoding is luminance (brightness). It is the sum of the signals coming from
the red cones, the green cones, the blue cones, and the rods. These provide the fine detail of
the image in black and white.
- A second form of encoding is used to separate blue and yellow (yellow being the sum of red
and green). The signal changes one way if blue is stronger than yellow, and changes in the
opposite way if yellow is stronger than blue.
- The third form of encoding is used to separate red and green. The signal changes one way
if green is stronger than red, and changes the other way if red is stronger than green.
The second and third kind of encoding do not have quite the fine resolution the luminance
encoding has. Black and white vision has finer detail than color vision.
In the fovea, where central vision occurs, each luminance ganglion cell receives signal from
only one cone cell of each color. There are no rod cells in the fovea, so it is night blind.
It is interesting that the light has to pass through the ganglion and bipolar cells to get to
the rods and cones. The retina seems to be made backward. But this might protect the light
sensitive cells from damage.
Note that the genes for the cone cells have two segments.
|
KINDS AND EFFECTS OF DEFECTIVE COLOR VISION
These are the major types of color vision defects:
- Protanopia - The loss of the red-sensitive cells
A person with protanopia sees in tints and shades of the colors yellow and blue. Red
objects look very dark.
- Deuteranopia - The loss of the green-sensitive cells
A person with deuteranopia sees in tints and shades of the colors yellow and blue. Green
objects are slightly darker than normal. This is also called deutanopia.
- Tritanopia - The loss of the blue-sensitive cells
A person with tritanopia sees in tints and shades of the colors red and green. Blue
objects look dark.
- Tetartanopia - Undocumented loss of sensitivity to yellow (see inset)
- Protanomaly - The red sensitive cells are sensitive to leaf green (yellow green) instead
of red
A person with protanomaly sees full color, but some oranges, yellows, greens, and browns
are seen as the wrong color.
- Deuteranomaly - The green sensitive cells are sensitive to yellow instead
A person with deuteranomaly sees full color, but some oranges, yellows, greens, and browns
are seen as the wrong color.
- Tritanomaly - The blue sensitive cells are sensitive to cyan instead
A person with tritanomaly sees full color, but some magentas, violets, blues, cyans, and
greens are seen as the wrong color.
- Red-Green Indistinction - No red-green differentiation but no sensitivity changes
A person with red-green indistinction sees in tints and shades of yellow and blue. All
objects are the correct brightness (no change in spectral sensitivity). Causes include
missing bipolar cells differentiating red and green, or red and green pigments mixed in the
same kind of cone cell.
- Red-Green anomaly - Both the red sensitive cells and the green sensitive cells respond to
the wrong colors.
A person with red-green anomaly sees color, but some reds, oranges, yellows, greens, and
browns are seen as the wrong color.
Red-green anomaly is protanomaly and deuteranomaly in the same person.
- Cone Monochromatism - Only the blue sensitive cells and the rods work
A person with cone monochromatism sees the world in black and white. Green and yellow
appear dark, and red appears black.
- Rod monochromatism - Only the rods work
A person with rod monochromatism sees the world in black and white. Reds appear black.
The person can not see well in bright light.
|
DEFECTIVE COLOR VISION
Outer ring:
Normal color vision
Second ring:
Protanopia
Third ring:
Deuteranopia
Inner ring:
Tritanopia
More Definitions
- Protan - Refers to any defect in the red cone, including protanopia and protanomaly
- Deutan - Refers to any defect in the green cone, including deuteranopia and deuteranomaly.
- Tritan - Refers to any defect in the blue cone, including tritanopia and tritanomaly.
Tetartanopia is very rare, if it exists at all. It might be a failure of the bipolar cells
for blue-yellow differentiation. Or it might have been an attempt to provide a missing disease
that the Hering Opponent Color theory predicted.
|
The genes for rod and cone cells have two segments. One segment encodes the color sensor
protein, the other segment tells the cell which bipolar cell nerve endings to connect to for
encoding lightness and color. There are several factors that interact to produce the kinds
of defective color vision observed:
- The blue cone cell gene (chromosome 7) and rod cell gene (chromosome 3) are in widely
separated places on different chromosomes.
- The red and green cone genes are next to each other on the same chromosome, making copying
errors more likely for those genes.
- The red and green genes are on the X chromosome. This makes a difference between men and
women for the kinds of observed defects in color vision.
- The bipolar cells cause defects in the sensors to be encoded as colors in certain ways.
- The defects could also be in the bipolar cells or ganglion cells (but this is rare if it
happens at all).
How color vision defects are interrelated:
- Defective color responses for red and for green vision are related, because the genes are
on the same chromosome.
- Defective color responses for red and for green vision are also sex-linked, because they
are on the X chromosome.
- There is a site on another chromosome that can remove all cone vision if it is damaged.
- Other than those, color vision defects are independent of each other.
- Defects that affect only red-green vision are independent of defects that affect only blue
vision.
There are several ways a gene can be altered to produce defective color vision:
- The gene can be damaged so it stops working.
- The gene for the color reception grid can be damaged, so the grid responds to the wrong
color.
- The color reception grid is copied from the wrong gene (usually this affects red and
green).
- Only part of the color reception grid is copied from the wrong gene.
- The wrong bipolar cell nerve connection is encoded in the gene.
- Multiple color reception grid types are placed into the same cones.
- A type of bipolar cell is missing or is connected wrong (rare if extant at all).
- A type of ganglion cell is missing or connected wrong (rare if extant at all).
Male
A gene is represented here in the following way:
---CONE
The letters CONE indicate the color reception grid,
the --- indicates the bipolar cell nerve endings to use.
The observed red-green color variants in men (one X chromosome) include the following:
Case | Gene Structure |
Seen as Red (peak) | Seen as Green (peak) |
Type of Defect | Observed Effects |
0 |
---CONE
---CONE |
(peak) | (peak) |
Normal Color vision |
Trichromatic vision |
1 |
---CONE
---CONE |
| (peak) |
Missing red cone gene |
Protanopia - Red blind (also called red-green blindness) |
2 |
---CONE
---CONE |
(peak) | (peak) |
Green pigments with red and green encoding |
Protanopia (variant) - Red blind (also called red-green blindness) |
3 |
---CONE
---CONE |
(peak) |
|
Green cone with red encoding |
Protanopia (variant) - Red blind (also called red-green blindness) |
4 |
---CONE
---CONE |
(peak) | (peak) |
Red cone is a yellow cone instead |
Protanomaly - Red weak |
5A |
---CONE
---CONE |
(peak) |
|
Yellow (red + green) cone with red encoding |
Red-Green Indistinction - Red-Green blind |
5B |
---CONE
---CONE |
(peak) | (peak) |
Red and green pigments with red encoding |
Red-Green Indistinction - Red-Green blind |
6A |
---CONE
---CONE |
| (peak) |
Yellow (red + green) cone with green encoding |
Red-Green Indistinction - Red-Green blind |
6B |
---CONE
---CONE |
| (peak) |
Red and green pigments with green encoding |
Red-Green Indistinction - Red-Green blind |
7 |
---CONE
---CONE |
(peak) | (peak) |
Green cone is a yellow cone instead |
Deuteranomaly - Green weak |
8 |
---CONE
---CONE |
| (peak) |
Red cone with green encoding |
Deuteranopia (variant) - Green blind (also called red-green blindness) |
9 |
---CONE
---CONE |
(peak) | (peak) |
Red pigments with red and green encoding |
Deuteranopia (variant) - Green blind (also called red-green blindness) |
10 |
---CONE
---CONE |
(peak) |
|
Missing green cone gene |
Deuteranopia - Green blind (also called red-green blindness) |
Since cases 5A and 5B are visually identical and have identical hereditary results, they can
be counted as case 5 for the rest of this page.
Since cases 6A and 6B are visually identical and have identical hereditary results, they can
be counted as case 6.
Cases 1, 2, and 3 produce the same visual result, but have different hereditary results. They
must be counted as separate cases.
Cases 8, 9, and 10 produce the same visual result, but have different hereditary results.
They must be counted as separate cases.
Note that the color "yellow" in cases 4, 5A, 6A, and 7 is generic, and can refer to
any color between leaf green and orange. The precise color depends on how much of each gene is
retained in the hybrid.
Female
The case for a female subject is more involved, because there are two X chromosomes. The
possible cases become the cross product of the two X chromosomes. But note that almost all cases
of defective color vision in females are in the top row or the left column. The others are rare.
This is a table of possible defective color vision in females. The first number is the visual
case. The number after the C means the female is a carrier of the numbered genes. The last line
shows possibilities for males:
X Mother Father |
---CONE 0
---CONE |
---CONE 1
---CONE |
---CONE 2
---CONE |
---CONE 3
---CONE |
---CONE 4
---CONE |
---CONE 5
---CONE |
---CONE 6
---CONE |
---CONE 7
---CONE |
---CONE 8
---CONE |
---CONE 9
---CONE |
---CONE 10
---CONE |
---CONE 0
---CONE |
0 Normal
(p)
(p) |
0 C:1
(p)
(p) |
4 C:2
(p)
(p) |
4 C:3
(p)
(p) |
11 C:4
(p)
(p) |
11 C:5
(p)
(p) |
12 C:6
(p)
(p) |
12 C:7
(p)
(p) |
7 C:8
(p)
(p) |
7 C:9
(p)
(p) |
0 C:10
(p)
(p) |
---CONE 1
---CONE |
0 C:1
(p)
(p) |
1 C:1
(p)
(p) |
2 C:1,2
(p)
(p) |
2 C:1,3
(p)
(p) |
4 C:1,4
(p)
(p) |
4 C:1,5
(p)
(p) |
13 C:1,6
(p)
(p) |
12 C:1,7
(p)
(p) |
6 C:1,8
(p)
(p) |
7 C:1.9
(p)
(p) |
0 C:1,10
(p)
(p) |
---CONE 2
---CONE |
4 C:2
(p)
(p) |
2 C:1,2
(p)
(p) |
2 C:2
(p)
(p) |
2 C:2,3
(p)
(p) |
15 C:2,4
(p)
(p) |
15 C:2,5
(p)
(p) |
15* C:2,6
(p)
(p) |
17 C:2,7
(p)
(p) |
4* C:2,8
(p)
(p) |
20 C:2,9
(p)
(p) |
4 C:2,10
(p)
(p) |
---CONE 3
---CONE |
4 C:3
(p)
(p) |
2 C:1,3
(p)
(p) |
2 C:2,3
(p)
(p) |
3 C:3
(p)
(p) |
15 C:3,4
(p)
(p) |
13* C:3,5
(p)
(p) |
4* C:3,6
(p)
(p) |
20 C:3,7
(p)
(p) |
0* C:3,8
(p)
(p) |
7* C:3,9
(p)
(p) |
5 C:3,10
(p)
(p) |
---CONE 4
---CONE |
11 C:4
(p)
(p) |
4 C:1,4
(p)
(p) |
15 C:2,4
(p)
(p) |
15 C:3,4
(p)
(p) |
4 C:4
(p)
(p) |
4 C:4,5
(p)
(p) |
17 C:4,6
(p)
(p) |
19 C:4,7
(p)
(p) |
20 C:4,8
(p)
(p) |
18 C:4,9
(p)
(p) |
11 C:4,10
(p)
(p) |
---CONE 5
---CONE |
11 C:5
(p)
(p) |
4 C:1,5
(p)
(p) |
15 C:2,5
(p)
(p) |
13* C:3,5
(p)
(p) |
4 C:4,5
(p)
(p) |
5 C:5
(p)
(p) |
20 C:5,6
(p)
(p) |
18 C:5,7
(p)
(p) |
7* C:5,8
(p)
(p) |
16* C:5,9
(p)
(p) |
14 C:5,10
(p)
(p) |
---CONE 6
---CONE |
12 C:6
(p)
(p) |
13 C:1,6
(p)
(p) |
15* C:2,6
(p)
(p) |
4* C:3,6
(p)
(p) |
17 C:4,6
(p)
(p) |
20 C:5,6
(p)
(p) |
6 C:6
(p)
(p) |
7 C:6,7
(p)
(p) |
14* C:6,8
(p)
(p) |
16 C:6,9
(p)
(p) |
7 C:6,10
(p)
(p) |
---CONE 7
---CONE |
12 C:7
(p)
(p) |
12 C:1,7
(p)
(p) |
17 C:2,7
(p)
(p) |
20 C:3,7
(p)
(p) |
19 C:4,7
(p)
(p) |
18 C:5,7
(p)
(p) |
7 C:6,7
(p)
(p) |
7 C:7
(p)
(p) |
16 C:7,8
(p)
(p) |
16 C:7,9
(p)
(p) |
7 C:7,10
(p)
(p) |
---CONE 8
---CONE |
7 C:8
(p)
(p) |
6 C:1,8
(p)
(p) |
4* C:2,8
(p)
(p) |
0* C:3,8
(p)
(p) |
20 C:4,8
(p)
(p) |
17* C:5,8
(p)
(p) |
14* C:6,8
(p)
(p) |
16 C:7,8
(p)
(p) |
8 C:8
(p)
(p) |
9 C:8,9
(p)
(p) |
9 C:8,10
(p)
(p) |
---CONE 9
---CONE |
7 C:9
(p)
(p) |
7 C:1,9
(p)
(p) |
20 C:2,9
(p)
(p) |
7* C:3,9
(p)
(p) |
18 C:4,9
(p)
(p) |
16* C:5,9
(p)
(p) |
16 C:6,9
(p)
(p) |
16 C:7,9
(p)
(p) |
9 C:8,9
(p)
(p) |
9 C:9
(p)
(p) |
9 C:9,10
(p)
(p) |
---CONE 10
---CONE |
0 C:10
(p)
(p) |
0 C:1,10
(p)
(p) |
4 C:2,10
(p)
(p) |
5 C:3,10
(p)
(p) |
11 C:4,10
(p)
(p) |
14 C:5,10
(p)
(p) |
7 C:6,10
(p)
(p) |
7 C:7,10
(p)
(p) |
9 C:8,10
(p)
(p) |
9 C:9,10
(p)
(p) |
10 C:10
(p)
(p) |
|
Male |
0 Normal
(p)
(p) |
1
(p)
(p) |
2
(p)
(p) |
3
(p)
(p) |
4
(p)
(p) |
5
(p)
(p) |
6
(p)
(p) |
7
(p)
(p) |
8
(p)
(p) |
9
(p)
(p) |
10
(p)
(p) |
* = The red cone and green cone functions are traded, with the red cone detecting a shorter
wavelength than the green cone does.
The extra visual anomalies found only in females are listed below. Other gene combinations
producing the same color sensations are possible:
Case | Gene Structure |
Seen as Red (peak) | Seen as Green (peak) |
Type of Defect | Observed Effects |
0* |
---CONE
---CONE
---CONE
---CONE |
(peak) | (peak) |
Normal color vision with red and green traded |
Trichromatic vision and double dichromat carrier
Maybe not aware of traded red and green functions. |
11 |
---CONE
---CONE
---CONE
---CONE |
(peak) | (peak) |
Red cone shifted to orange |
Protanomaly (variant) |
12 |
---CONE
---CONE
---CONE
---CONE |
(peak) | (peak) |
Green cone shifted to leaf |
Deuteranomaly (variant) |
13 |
---CONE
---CONE
---CONE
---CONE |
(peak) | (peak) |
Green cone shifted to leaf, no red cone |
Deuteranomalous Protanopia - Red blind, green defective |
14 |
---CONE
---CONE
---CONE
---CONE |
(peak) | (peak) |
Red cone shifted to orange, no green cone. |
Protanomalous Deuteranopia - Green blind, red defective |
15 |
---CONE
---CONE
---CONE
---CONE |
(peak) | (peak) |
Red cone shifted to leaf |
Protanomaly (variant) |
16 |
---CONE
---CONE
---CONE
---CONE |
(peak) | (peak) |
Green cone shifted to orange |
Deuteranomaly (variant) |
17 |
---CONE
---CONE
---CONE
---CONE |
(peak) | (peak) |
Red cone shifted to yellow, green cone shifted to leaf |
Red-Green Anomaly (variant) |
18 |
---CONE
---CONE
---CONE
---CONE |
(peak) | (peak) |
Red cone shifted to orange, green cone shifted to yellow |
Red-Green Anomaly (variant) |
19 |
---CONE
---CONE
---CONE
---CONE |
(peak) | (peak) |
Red cone shifted to orange, green cone shifted to leaf |
Red-Green Anomaly (variant) |
20 |
---CONE
---CONE
---CONE
---CONE |
(peak) | (peak) |
Red cone and green cone both shifted to yellow |
Red-Green Indistinction |
What about tetrachromacy in females?
Some of the above cases of protanomaly and deuteranomaly could possibly produce eyes with 4
different color sensitive pigments. But what kind of vision this produces depends on how the
pigments are distributed and what kind of nerve connections to the brain are created:
- Case 1: One kind of cone has two pigments. Three neural pathways.
In this case, the response curve of the cone is altered, making the vision system
ultrasensitive to color changes in certain parts of the spectrum that normal people can't
distinguish, but insensitive to color differences other people notice.
- Case 2: Four kinds of cones, but two are identically connected to the nervous system. Three
neural pathways.
In this case, the response curve of the combination of cones is altered, making the vision
system ultrasensitive to color changes in certain parts of the spectrum that normal people
can't distinguish, but insensitive to color differences other people notice.
- Case 3: Four kinds of cones. Four neural pathways, with an extra differencing network for
the extra colors.
In this case, there is a genuine increase in the number of colors visible to the person.
This system can notice changes in the spectral emission curve of the light source, including
spectral gaps not noticed by other people. It makes the vision system ultrasensitive to color
changes in certain parts of the spectrum that normal people can't distinguish, but not
reducing sensitivity to color differences other people notice.
Note that adding a human gene for a green cone to mice (dichromatic, blue and yellow) did
not give them trichromatic vision. It made two kinds of cone cells, but did not make a
differencing network to tell the difference between green and red.
|
DESIGNING SYSTEMS TO COMPENSATE FOR DEFECTIVE COLOR VISION
Signs, Especially Traffic Signs
Since most signs are made for luminance contrast, reading the sign is not a problem. Only if
some kind of color code is built into the sign does defective color vision come into play.
One place where a color code is built into the sign is a traffic sign. The following color
code is used:
- White - Regulatory sign, lettering on all signs
except orange, yellow, yellow-green. and white
- Black - Special Regulatory sign, lettering on
signs that are orange, yellow, yellow-green or white
- Strong Magenta - Emergency traffic control signs
- Red - Stop, Yield, Do not enter, and Wrong way
- Orange - Construction and temporary traffic
control
- Yellow - Warning of hazard
- Strong yellow green - School crossing or children
present
- Green - Guidance to locations, permitted
movements
- Blue - Road user services guidance, evacuation
routes
- Violet - Automatic toll collection
- Brown - Recreation and cultural interest
guidance
Only a few cases exist where color identification might cause confusion, because traffic signs
also have shape codes:
- Orange and red - No signs exist that could be confused.
- Red and magenta - No signs exist that could be confused.
- Orange and magenta - The signs have the same meanings. The only difference is the duration
of the traffic event.
- Orange and yellow - The signs have the same meanings. The only difference is the duration
of the traffic event.
- Yellow and yellow green - The signs have the same meanings. The only difference is the extra
meaning of children present.
- Orange and green - No signs exist that could be confused.
- Yellow and green - No signs exist that could be confused.
- Brown and green - The signs have the same meanings. The only difference is the recreational
or cultural content.
- Brown and blue - The signs have the same meanings. The only difference is the service,
recreational, or cultural content.
Traffic Signals
Because recognizing the aspect of the traffic signal is so important to traffic safety,
special engineering is needed to ensure that drivers with defective color vision receive the
correct message. Several methods are used:
- For anomalous color vision, use the special glasses or filters below.
- Position of the light in the signal face.
- Selection of colors that can still be distinguished from each other by most people with
defective color vision. This has been done in the US DOT standards.
- Other methods, such as different shapes for different colors.
Color Filters, glasses, or lamps for Identifying Basic Colors
A trick that can be used (but not while driving a car) is to have color filters to hold in
front of the eye. Choice of filters depends on the type of color blindness:
- For protanopia and protanomaly, use a red filter and a green filter.
Only magenta, red, orange, yellow, white, and some violet pass through the red filter.
Only cyan, green, yellow, white, and some orange pass through the green filter.
A magenta and a cyan filter can be substituted for the red and green filter.
- For deuteranopia, deuteranomaly, and red-green indistinction, use a red filter and a green
filter.
Only magenta, red, orange, yellow, white, and some violet pass through the red filter.
Only cyan, green, yellow, white, and some orange pass through the green filter.
A magenta and a cyan filter can be substituted for the red and green filter.
- For tritanopia and tritanomaly, use a blue filter and a yellow filter.
Only magenta, red, orange, yellow, white, green, and cyan pass through the yellow filter.
Only cyan, blue, violet, magenta, and white pass through the blue filter.
- For monochromatism, use a red filter, a green filter, and a blue filter.
Only magenta, red, orange, yellow, and some violet pass through the red filter. The red
may be very dark.
Only cyan, green, yellow, and some orange pass through the green filter.
Only cyan, blue, violet, magenta, and white pass through the blue filter.
A magenta, a yellow, and a cyan filter can be substituted.
- Filters of different colors can be used in the left and right lenses, sacrificing depth
perception for color differentiation.
A trick that can be used is to have colored lights to shine on object to help discern its
color. Choice of lamp colors depends on the type of color blindness:
- Use colored lights that can be switched on to make certain colors stand out.
- Use lights of different colors on opposite sides of the work station.
- Use a blinking colored light to make certain colors flash.
- Place a rotating color wheel in front of a light to make certain colors blink differently
than other colors.
Special Lamps for Anomalous Color Vision
This trick can be used with protanomaly, deuteranomaly, and tritanomaly. A special lamp
can be used to let the person see what others see, under certain conditions:
- The special lamp must emit only red, green, and blue light, but no other colors.
- The lamp can contain separate bulbs for red, green, and blue.
- A tricolor (red, green, blue) LED can be used.
- The special lamps must be the only illumination in the area.
- The person may have to learn what each of the colors looks like under the special lamp.
- This works because most objects that are orange, yellow, cyan, magenta, or white reflect
the colors red, green, and blue in the proper ratios.
- The colors seen here will be the same colors seen on a tricolor color TV screen.
- This method might be useful for identifying colors in color coding systems.
- Build Special Lamps for Defective Color Vision.
Special Glasses and filters for Anomalous Color Vision
This trick can be used with protanomaly, deuteranomaly, and tritanopia. A special
filter can be used to let the person see what others see, under certain conditions:
- The special filter removes spectral yellow and spectral cyan from the light seen by the
viewer.
- The best filter is a notch filter with sharp edges.
- The illumination must contain the colors the filters pass.
- The person may have to learn what each of the colors looks like using the glasses.
- This works because most objects that are orange, yellow, cyan, magenta, or white reflect
the colors red, green, and blue in the proper ratios.
- The colors seen here will be the same colors seen on a color TV screen.
- This method is useful for identifying colors in color coding systems.
- This method will probably work for identifying signal lights for trains, automobiles,
boats, ships, and aircraft.
- As long as the driver can identify the signals with them on, it is safe to drive a car
with these glasses.
- This will NOT work for protanopia, deuteranopia, tritanopia, red-green indistinction,
or total color blindness.
- The glasses must be removed when driving under low-pressure sodium street lights. The
filters remove all of the yellow light these streetlights make.
- A company named EnChroma makes these glasses.
Photography and Television for Anomalous Color Vision
This trick can be used with protanomaly, deuteranomaly, and tritanomaly. The process
of tricolor color photography or television can be used to let the person see what others
see, under certain conditions:
- The method of color separation in tricolor photography and color television divides the
response into red, green, and blue signals.
- Display of the image on a TV set, a camera screen, or a monitor provides the spectral
gaps necessary for producing separate red, green, and blue stimuli that can work with the
anomalous cone cells.
- A Quattron 4-color screen can NOT be used for this.
- This would NOT work with any color photography system that relies on spectral
interference or separation instead of color separation (e.g. Lippman Photochrome).
- The cyan-magenta-yellow printing process may or may not work with individual people.
This depends on the nature of the pigments printed and the specific kind of anomalous vision
present. But it should work with the glasses found above.
- The person may have to learn what each of the colors looks like on the screen and in the
prints.
- This works because most objects that are orange, yellow, cyan, magenta, or white reflect
the colors red, green, and blue in the proper ratios.
- This could be useful for identifying colors in color coding systems. Point a camera at
the color to be identified and look at the monitor or the screen on a camera.
Color Coded Wiring and Other Color Codes
Various tricks work to make color codes work:
- Choose sets of colors that are easily distinguished with protanopic, deuteranopic, and
tritanopic vision. Examples:
Colors |
Magenta Yellow Cyan Black Gray |
Orange White Azure Black Gray |
Normal |
|
|
Protan |
|
|
Deutan |
|
|
Tritan |
|
|
- Use the color filter trick above.
- Use a colored mirror.
- Use colored lights that can be switched on to make certain colors stand out.
- Use lights of different colors on opposite sides of the work station.
- For anomalous color vision, use the special lamp above.
- For anomalous color vision, use the special glasses or filters above.
- For anomalous color vision, use the camera method above.
- Use a blinking colored light to make certain colors flash.
- Place a rotating color wheel in front of a light to make certain colors blink differently
than other colors.
- If only certain colors are in use, maybe all that is needed are samples of those colors on
a card with their names.
Colored Lights on Equipment
Various tricks work to distinguish the colors:
- Place a small colored mirror where it will reflect the signal light.
- For anomalous color vision, use the special glasses or filters above.
- Use the color filter trick above.
Colored Lights on Aircraft, Airport Signals, Boats, Ships, and Coast Guard Markers
Various tricks work to distinguish the colors:
- For anomalous color vision, use the special glasses or filters above.
- Place a small colored mirror nearby, where it can reflect the light from the source into
the eye of the operator.
- Use the color filter trick above. A flat filter mounted on eyeglass frames that can swivel
down in a hurry is useful here.
Suggested modifications to the light colors:
- Since no aviation or marine lights are yellow, the red vs yellow difficulty with
automotive signals and deuteranopia is not a problem. The only light colors used are red,
white, and green.
- The color standards for aircraft and marine uses should be the same ones used for
traffic signals.
- The white should be white, neither bluish nor yellowish. It must be a continuous spectrum,
not a high intensity discharge or LED source with gaps in the spectrum.
- The red must contain orange, but no yellow, green, or blue. This looks yellow to people
with protanopia and deuteranopia or red to people with tritanopia.
- The green must contain cyan and blue, but no red, orange, or yellow. This looks blue to
people with protanopia and deuteranopia, or green to people with tritanopia.
If the lamps do not follow this standard, government is committing a serious error by not
adopting it.
- Because the lights on these objects can be a considerable distance away, shapes and colored
insertions used on traffic signals will not work here.
- The only people not addressed by this standard are those with monochromatism.
Colored Lights on Trains, Train Signals, and Other Ground Transportation Equipment
Various tricks work to distinguish the colors:
- For anomalous color vision, use the special glasses or filters above.
- Place a small colored mirror nearby, where it can reflect the light from the source into
the eye of the operator.
- Use the color filter trick above. A flat filter mounted on eyeglass frames that can swivel
down in a hurry is useful here.
Suggested modifications to the light colors:
- The color standards for train and other ground transportation lights should be the same
ones used for
traffic signals. So the same problems and solutions apply.
- Semaphore, position light, and color position light signals do not depend on color
vision.
|